Jump to content

Wagiman language: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Citation bot (talk | contribs)
Add: date. Removed parameters. Some additions/deletions were actually parameter name changes. | You can use this bot yourself. Report bugs here. | Activated by AManWithNoPlan | All pages linked from User:AManWithNoPlan/sandbox2 | via #UCB_webform_linked
(30 intermediate revisions by 21 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{distinguish|Wagaman language (disambiguation){{!}}Languages called Wagaman or Wakaman}}
{{short description|Indigenous Australian language}}
{{short description|Indigenous Australian language}}
{{distinguish|Wagaman language (disambiguation){{!}}Wagaman language}}
{{Use Australian English|date=August 2018}}
{{Use Australian English|date=August 2018}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=August 2018}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=August 2018}}
Line 12: Line 12:
|ref=<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.ethnologue.com/language/waq|title=Wageman|work=Ethnologue|access-date=2018-06-10|language=en}}</ref>
|ref=<ref>{{Cite news|url=https://www.ethnologue.com/language/waq|title=Wageman|work=Ethnologue|access-date=2018-06-10|language=en}}</ref>
|familycolor=Australian
|familycolor=Australian
|family=[[language isolate]]<ref name=Bowern/>
|family=[[Language isolate]]<ref name=Bowern/>
|iso3=waq
|iso3=waq
|aiatsis=N27
|aiatsis=N27
Line 21: Line 21:
|notice=IPA
|notice=IPA
}}
}}
'''Wagiman''', also spelt '''Wageman''', '''Wakiman''', '''Wogeman''',<ref>Carrington, L., & Triffitt, G. (1999: 266)</ref> and other variants, is a near-extinct [[Aboriginal Australian language]] spoken by a small number of [[Wagiman people]]<ref name="Gordon, R. G., Jr. 2005">Gordon, R. G., Jr. (2005)</ref><ref name=AIATSIS/> in and around [[Pine Creek, Northern Territory|Pine Creek]], in the [[Katherine, Northern Territory|Katherine]] Region of the [[Northern Territory]].

'''Wagiman''' (also spelled ''Wageman'', ''Wakiman'', ''Wogeman'', ''Wakaman'')<ref>Carrington, L., & Triffitt, G. (1999: 266)</ref> is a near-extinct [[indigenous Australian language]] spoken by fewer than 10 [[Wagiman people]]<ref name="Gordon, R. G., Jr. 2005">Gordon, R. G., Jr. (2005)</ref> in and around [[Pine Creek, Northern Territory|Pine Creek]], in the [[Katherine, Northern Territory|Katherine]] Region of the [[Northern Territory]].


The Wagiman language is notable within linguistics for its complex system of verbal morphology, which remains under-investigated, its possession of a cross-linguistically rare part of speech called a [[coverb]], its [[#Complex predicates|complex predicates]] and for its ability to productively verbalise coverbs.
The Wagiman language is notable within linguistics for its complex system of verbal morphology, which remains under-investigated, its possession of a cross-linguistically rare part of speech called a [[coverb]], its [[#Complex predicates|complex predicates]] and for its ability to productively verbalise coverbs.


Wagiman is expected to become extinct within the first half of the century, as the youngest generation of Wadjigan people speak no Wagiman at all, and understand very little.<ref name="Cook, A.R. 1987: 17-19">Cook, A.R. (1987: 17-19)</ref>
As of 1999 Wagiman was expected to become extinct within the next generation, as the youngest generation spoke no Wagiman and understood very little.<ref name="Wilson, S. 1999">Wilson, S. (1999)</ref> The [[2011 Australian census]] recorded 30 speakers, while the [[2016 Australian census]] recorded 18 speakers.<ref name=AIATSIS/>


==Language and speakers==
==Language and speakers==
===Relation with other languages===
===Relation with other languages===
[[Image:Jaben.jpg|thumb|right|300px|Jaben; the [[Chlamydosaurus|Frilled-Neck Lizard]], one of the Wagiman people's sacred sites]]
[[Image:Jaben.jpg|thumb|right|300px|Jaben; the [[Chlamydosaurus|Frilled-Neck Lizard]], one of the Wagiman people's sacred sites]]
Wagiman is a [[language isolate]].<ref name=Bowern>Bowern, C. (2011)</ref> It was once assumed to be a member of the adjacent [[Gunwinyguan languages|Gunwinyguan]] family that stretches from [[Arnhem Land]], throughout [[Kakadu National Park]] and south to Katherine<ref>Merlan, F.C. (1994: 3-4)</ref>, but this has since been rejected. Wagiman may still bear a remote relation with its neighbouring languages but this is yet to be demonstrated.<ref name="Evans 2003">{{Citation | author1=Evans, Nicholas | title=The non-Pama-Nyungan languages of northern Australia : comparative studies of the continent's most linguistically complex region | date=2003 | publisher=Pacific Linguistics | isbn=978-0-85883-538-2 | pages=14}}</ref>
Wagiman is a [[language isolate]] within the hypothetical Australian language family.<ref name=Bowern>Bowern, C. (2011)</ref> It was once assumed to be a member of the adjacent [[Gunwinyguan languages|Gunwinyguan]] family that stretches from [[Arnhem Land]], throughout [[Kakadu National Park]] and south to Katherine,<ref name="Merlan, F.C. 1994: 3-4">Merlan, F.C. (1994: 3-4)</ref> but this has since been rejected. Wagiman may still bear a remote relation with its neighbouring languages but this is yet to be demonstrated.<ref name="Evans 2003">{{Citation | author1=Evans, Nicholas | title=The non-Pama-Nyungan languages of northern Australia : comparative studies of the continent's most linguistically complex region | date=2003 | publisher=Pacific Linguistics | isbn=978-0-85883-538-2 | pages=14}}</ref>


Francesca Merlan believes that Wagiman may be distantly related to the [[Wardaman language|Yangmanic languages]], citing that they both use [[coverb|verbal particles]] in a similar way, to the exclusion of neighbouring languages (such as [[Jawoyn language|Jawoyn]] and [[Mangarrayi language|Mangarrayi]]).<ref>Merlan, F.C. (1994: 3-4)</ref> Stephen Wilson additionally notes some other similarities, such as in the pronominal prefixes and the marking of non-case-marked nominals. However both languages have a very low cognacy rate (shared vocabulary) of about 10%. Wagiman is also superficially similar to the neighbouring [[Gunwinyguan languages]] phonologically (both share a fortis/lenis stop contrast and a phonemic [[glottal stop]]) and to the [[Mirndi languages|Mirndi language]] [[Jaminjung language|Jaminjung-Ngaliwurru]] in the use of coverbs.<ref>Wilson, S. (1999: 5-6)</ref> Mark Harvey notes similarities in the verbal inflectional systems between Wagiman and the neighbouring [[Eastern Daly languages]].<ref name="Harvey 2003"> Harvey, M. (2003) "Verb systems in the Eastern Daly language family." In Nicholas Evans, ed. ''The Non-Pama-Nyungan languages of northern Australia.''</ref>
Francesca Merlan believes that Wagiman may be distantly related to the [[Wardaman language|Yangmanic languages]], citing that they both use [[coverb|verbal particles]] in a similar way, to the exclusion of neighbouring languages (such as [[Jawoyn language|Jawoyn]] and [[Mangarrayi language|Mangarrayi]]).<ref name="Merlan, F.C. 1994: 3-4"/> Stephen Wilson additionally notes some other similarities, such as in the pronominal prefixes and the marking of non-case-marked nominals. However both languages have a very low cognacy rate (shared vocabulary) of about 10%. Wagiman is also superficially similar to the neighbouring [[Gunwinyguan languages]] phonologically (both share a fortis/lenis stop contrast and a phonemic [[glottal stop]]) and to the [[Mirndi languages|Mirndi language]] [[Jaminjung language|Jaminjung-Ngaliwurru]] in the use of coverbs.<ref>Wilson, S. (1999: 5-6)</ref> Mark Harvey notes similarities in the verbal inflectional systems between Wagiman and the neighbouring [[Eastern Daly languages]].<ref name="Harvey 2003">Harvey, M. (2003) "Verb systems in the Eastern Daly language family." In Nicholas Evans, ed. ''The Non-Pama-Nyungan languages of northern Australia.''</ref>


===Speakers===
===Speakers===
Wagiman is the ancestral language of the Wagiman people, [[Aboriginal Australians]] whose traditional land, before colonisation, extended for hundreds of square kilometres from the [[Stuart Highway]], throughout the Mid-Daly Basin, and across the [[Daly River]].<ref>Wilson, A. (2006: 5)</ref> The land is highly fertile and well-watered, and contains a number of cattle stations, on which many members of the ethnic group used to work. These stations include Claravale, Dorisvale, Jindare, Oolloo and Douglas.<ref>Wilson, S. (1999: 5)</ref>
Wagiman is the ancestral language of the Wagiman people, [[Aboriginal Australians]] whose traditional land, before colonisation, extended for hundreds of square kilometres from the [[Stuart Highway]], throughout the Mid-Daly Basin, and across the [[Daly River (Northern Territory)|Daly River]].<ref>Wilson, A. (2006: 5)</ref> The land is highly fertile and well-watered, and contains a number of cattle stations, on which many members of the ethnic group used to work. These stations include Claravale, Dorisvale, Jindare, Oolloo and Douglas.<ref>Wilson, S. (1999: 5)</ref>


The language region borders [[Waray language (Australia)|Waray]] to the north, [[Mayali language|Mayali]] (Gunwinygu) and [[Jawoyn language|Jawoyn]] on the east, [[Wardaman language|Wardaman]] and [[Jaminjung language|Jaminjung]] on the south, and [[Murrinh-patha language|Murrinh-Patha]], [[Ngan'giwumirri language|Ngan'giwumirri]] and [[Malak Malak language|Malak Malak]] on the west. Before colonisation, the lands surrounding Pine Creek, extending north to Brock's Creek, were traditionally associated with another language group that is now extinct, believed to have been [[Wulwulam language|Wulwulam]].<ref>Harvey, M. (2003: 295-97)</ref>
The language region borders [[Waray language (Australia)|Waray]] to the north, [[Mayali dialect|Mayali]] (Kunwinjku) and [[Jawoyn language|Jawoyn]] on the east, [[Wardaman language|Wardaman]] and [[Jaminjung language|Jaminjung]] on the south, and [[Murrinh-patha language|Murrinh-Patha]], [[Ngan'giwumirri language|Ngan'giwumirri]] and [[Malak Malak language|Malak Malak]] on the west. Before colonisation, the lands surrounding Pine Creek, extending north to Brock's Creek, were traditionally associated with another language group that is now extinct, believed to have been [[Wulwulam language|Wulwulam]].<ref>Harvey, M. (2003: 295-97)</ref>


The dominant language of the region is Mayali, a dialect of [[Bininj Kunwok]] traditionally associated with the region surrounding [[Maningrida, Northern Territory|Maningrida]], in Western Arnhem Land.<ref>Evans, N. (2003: 6-9)</ref> As it is a strong language with hundreds of speakers and a high rate of child acquisition, members of the Wagiman ethnic group gradually ceased teaching the Wagiman language to their children. As a result, many Wagiman people speak Mayali, while only a handful of [[Aboriginal elder|elders]] continue to speak Wagiman.{{cn|date=October 2021}}
===Current situation===
The dominant language of the region is Mayali, a dialect of [[Bininj Kunwok language|Bininj Gun-Wok]] traditionally associated with the region surrounding [[Maningrida, Northern Territory|Maningrida]], in Western Arnhem Land.<ref>Evans, N. (2003: 6-9)</ref> As it is a strong language with hundreds of speakers and a high rate of child acquisition, members of the Wagiman ethnic group gradually ceased teaching the Wagiman language to their children. As a result, many Wagiman people speak Mayali, while only a handful of elders, possibly no more than six, speak Wagiman.


The adults in the community are considered [[semi-speaker]]s as they have a passive understanding of Wagiman and generally only know a few basic words. Their children, the grandchildren of the elders, understand very little Wagiman and speak none.<ref name="Cook, A.R. 1987: 17-19"/> Apart from Mayali, [[Australian Kriol language|Kriol]], a [[creole language]] based on the vocabulary of [[English language|English]], is the ''[[lingua franca]]'' of the area. All members of the Wagiman ethnic group, as well as all other ethnic groups represented in the area, are native speakers of a moderate grade of Kriol.<ref name="Gordon, R. G., Jr. 2005"/>
In 1987 it was found that adults in the community understood the Wagiman language to a certain extent or knew only a few basic words, but speak Daly River [[Australian Kriol|Kriol]] as their daily language. The youngest generation understood very little Wagiman and spoke none.<ref name="Cook, A.R. 1987: 17-19">Cook, A.R. (1987: 17-19)</ref> As of 1999 Wagiman was expected to become extinct within the next generation, as the youngest generation spoke no Wagiman and understood very little.<ref name="Wilson, S. 1999"/> In 2005 only 10 speakers were recorded,<ref name="Gordon, R. G., Jr. 2005">Gordon, R. G., Jr. (2005)</ref> but the [[2011 Australian census]] recorded 30 speakers, with the [[2016 Australian census]] recording 18 speakers.<ref name=AIATSIS/>


The small number of Wagiman speakers are also partial speakers of a number of other languages besides Mayali, including Jaminjung, Wardaman and Dagoman.
Apart from Mayali, [[Australian Kriol language|Kriol]], a [[creole language]] based on the vocabulary of [[English language|English]], is the ''[[lingua franca]]'' of the area.<ref name="Gordon, R. G., Jr. 2005"/> The Wagiman people are also partial speakers of a number of other languages, including [[Jaminjung language|Jaminjung]], [[Wardaman language|Wardaman]] and [[Dagoman language|Dagoman]].{{cn|date=October 2021}}


===Dialects===
===Dialects===
Wagiman speakers are conscious of a distinction between two [[dialect]]s of Wagiman, which they refer to as ''matjjin no-roh-ma'' 'light language' and ''matjjin gu-nawutj-jan'' 'heavy language'. The differences are minor and speakers have no difficulty understanding one another.<ref name=wod>Wilson, S. (2001)</ref>
Wagiman speakers are conscious of a distinction between two [[dialect]]s of Wagiman, which they refer to as {{Lang|waq|matjjin norohma}} 'light language' and {{Lang|waq|matjjin gunawutjjan}} 'heavy language'. The differences are minor and speakers have no difficulty understanding one another.<ref name=wod>Wilson, S. (2001)</ref>


==Phonology and orthography==
==Wagiman grammar==
The Wagiman phonemic inventory is quite typical for a northern Australian language. It has six [[place of articulation|places of articulation]] with a [[stop consonant|stop]] and a [[nasal stop|nasal]] in each. There are also a number of [[lateral consonant|laterals]] and [[approximant consonant|approximants]], a [[trill consonant|trill]] and a phonemic [[glottal stop]] (represented in the orthography by 'h'). Wagiman also has a vowel inventory that is standard for the north of Australia, with a system of five vowels.
''All grammatical information from Wilson, S. (1999)''<ref>Wilson, S. (1999)</ref> ''unless otherwise noted.''


===Parts of speech===
===Consonants===
The three most important parts of speech in Wagiman are [[verb]]s, [[coverb]]s and [[nominal (linguistics)|nominals]]. Apart from these, there are a multitude of verbal and nominal [[affix]]es, [[interjection]]s and other particles. [[Pronoun]]s class with nominals.

====Nominals====
Like many Australian languages, Wagiman does not categorially distinguish [[noun]]s from [[adjective]]s. These form one word class that is called nominals. Wagiman nominals take case suffixes (see [[#Nominals 2|below]]) that denote their grammatical or semantic role in the sentence. The grammatical cases are ergative and absolutive, and the semantic cases include instrumental (using), allative (towards), ablative (from), locative (at), comitative (with, having), privative (without, lacking), temporal (at the time of) and semblative (resembling). The dative case can be either grammatical or semantic, depending on the syntactic requirements of the verb.

[[Demonstratives]] are similarly considered nominals in Wagiman, and take the same case suffixes depending on their semantic and syntactic roles; their function within the sentence. That is, the demonstrative ''mahan'' 'this', or 'here' (root: ''mayh-''), may take case just like any other nominal.
*''mayh-yi'' this-[[Ergative case|{{sc|erg}}]] 'this one (did it)'
*''mayh-ga'' this-[[Allative case|{{sc|all}}]] 'to here'

=====Examples of nominals=====
*''guda'' 'fire', 'wood' {{IPA|[ɡʊda]}}
*''wirin'' 'tree', 'stick' {{IPA|[wɪɻɪn]}}
*''lagiban'' 'man' {{IPA|[laɡɪban]}}
*''gordal'' 'head' {{IPA|[ɡɔɖaɫ]}}
*''lagiriny'' 'tail' {{IPA|[laɡɪɻɪɲ]}}
*''manyngardal'' 'tongue' {{IPA|[maɲŋaɖaɫ]}}

====Pronouns====
Pronouns are typologically nominals also, yet their [[morphosyntactic alignment]] is [[nominative–accusative language|nominative–accusative]] rather than ergative–absolutive.

{| class="wikitable"
!
![[Nominative case|Nominative]]
![[Accusative case|Accusative]]
![[Genitive case|Genitive]]
|-
!1st Singular
|''ngagun''
|''nganung''
|''nganing-gin''
|-
!1st Dual
|''nginyang''
|''nginyang''
|''nginyang-gin''
|-
!1st Plural [[inclusive and exclusive we|incl.]]
|''ngego''
|''ngerre-ngana''
|''gerdo-gin''
|-
!1st Plural [[inclusive and exclusive we|excl.]]
|''ngego''
|''ngerreju''
|''gerdo-gin''
|-
!2nd Singular
|''ngigun''
|''ngonggo''
|''ngonggo-gin''
|-
!2nd person Plural
|''ngogo''
|''ngorruju''
|''gordo-gin''
|-
!3rd Singular
|''ngonggega'' (rare)
|''nung''
|''nung-gin''
|-
!3rd Plural
|''bogo'' (rare)
|''borruju''
|''borro-gin''
|}

The 3rd person singular and plural nominative forms, ''ngonggega'' and ''bogo'', are labeled 'rare' because they are gradually becoming disused. Speakers prefer to use non-personal pronouns such as ''gayh-'' 'that' or ''gayh-gorden'' 'those'. Moreover, since the person and number of the subject is contained in the prefix of the verb, nominative free pronouns are often dropped.

=====Tripartite alignment=====
[[Image:Ergative alignment.svg|thumb|right|250px|Ergative alignment of the case system]]
While the nominal case system distinguishes the ergative case from absolutive, the free pronouns distinguish nominative from accusative, as shown above. However, they inflect for ergative case as well, resulting in a [[Tripartite language|tripartite]] case system, as in the following:

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
|ngagun-yi ngonggo ngany-bu-ng
|1SG.NOM-ERG 2SG.ACC {{gcl|1sgA|1st-person singular agent}}.{{gcl|2sgO|second-person singular object}}-hit-PFV
|'I hit you.'}}

[[Image:Accusative alignment.svg|thumb|right|250px|Accusative alignment of the free pronoun system. <br>A = Transitive agent <br>S = Intransitive argument <br> O = Transitive object]]
The nominative pronoun root in this instance, ''ngagun'' 'I', takes the ergative case suffix ''-yi'' to denote the fact that it is the agent of a transitive clause. Conversely, the same pronoun does not take the ergative case when acting as the argument of an intransitive clause:

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
|ngagun maman nga-yu
|1SG.NOM good 1SG-be.PRS
|'I am good.'}}

The accusative pronouns on the other hand, may be accusative or dative, depending on the syntactic requirements of the verb. In the traditional terminology, these pronouns can be either direct or indirect objects.

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
|ngagun-yi nga-nanda-yi nung
|1SG.NOM-ERG {{gcl|1sgA|1st-person singular "agent"}}.{{gcl|3sgO|3rd-person singular "object"}}-see-PST 3SG.ACC
|'I saw him/her.'}}

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
|nga-nawu-ndi wahan nung
|{{gcl|1sgA|1st-person singular agent}}.{{gcl|3sgO|3rd-person singular object}}-give-PST water 3SG.ACC
|'I gave the water to him/her.'}}

For these reasons, the pronouns are also labeled ''base'' for nominative–ergative pronouns, and ''oblique'' for accusative–dative pronouns.

=====Genitive pronouns=====
In the table above, genitive pronouns all end with ''-gin'', which is separated orthographically by a hyphen that normally divides morphemes. The ''-gin'' form here is not a separate morpheme and cannot be lexically segmented; there is no such word as ''nganing'' that would be formed by removing ''-gin'' from ''nganing-gin'' 'my/mine'. The fact that the genitive forms have regular endings across the entire pronoun paradigm may have been a historical accident.

This cannot be a nominal suffix like those listed above, since it may not attach to other nominals (*''warren-gin lari'' 'the child's hand'). Furthermore, the genitive pronouns may take a further case suffix, as in the example:
:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
|gorun nganing-gin-ba
|house my-ALL
|'To my house.'}}
This would be prohibited by the restriction against [[case stacking]] in Wagiman if the genitive ''-gin'' were a case suffix.

====Verbs====
Verbs are a class of word in Wagiman which contains fewer than 50 members. As it is a closed class, no more verbs are possible. They are often monosyllabic verb roots and all are vowel-final. Wagiman verbs obligatorily inflect for person and number of core arguments, and for the tense and aspect of the clause. A small set of verbs may take a non-finite suffix ''-yh'', in which it may not be further inflected for person or tense. That non-finite verb must then co-occur with another [[auxiliary verb]].

=====Examples of verbs=====
Each verb is listed with its past tense marker, which is the second morpheme. Pronunciation given where appropriate.
*''bu-ni'' 'hit'
*''di-nya'' 'come'
*''ra-ndi'' 'throw'
*''rinyi-ra'' 'fall'
*''nanda-yi'' 'see' {{IPA|[nandaɪ]}}
*''yu-nginy'' 'be' {{IPA|[jʊŋɪɲ]}}

====Coverbs====
There are so far over 500 recorded coverbs in Wagiman, and more are discovered with continuing research. Compared with verbs, coverbs are far more numerous and far more semantically rich. Verbs express simple, broad meanings such as ''yu-'' 'be', ''ya-'' 'go' and ''di-'' 'come', while coverbs convey more specific, semantically narrow meanings such as ''barnhbarn-na'' 'make footprints', ''lerdongh-nga'' 'play (a [[didjeridu]])' or ''murr-ma'' 'wade through shallow water using your feet to search for something'.

Coverbs however, cannot inflect for person and cannot, in themselves, head finite clauses. If they are to act as the head of a clause, they must combine with a verb, thereby forming a bipartite verbal compound, commonly called a [[#Complex predicates|complex predicate]].

=====Examples of coverbs=====
Each is listed with the ''-ma'' suffix (or its allomorph), which signals aspectual unmarkedness.
*''liri-ma'' 'swim' {{IPA|[lɪɻɪma]}}
*''dabale-ma'' '(go) around)' {{IPA|[dabalema]}}
*''gorrh-ma'' 'fish' {{IPA|[ɡɔrʔma]}}
*''dippart-ta'' 'jump' {{IPA|[dɪbˑaɖˑa]}}
*''wirrnh-na'' 'whistle' {{IPA|[wɪrʔna]}}

===Phonology and orthography===
The Wagiman phonemic inventory is quite typical for a northern Australian language. It has six [[place of articulation|places of articulation]] with a [[stop consonant|stop]] and a [[nasal stop|nasal]] in each. There are also a number of [[lateral consonant|laterals]] and [[approximant consonant|approximants]], a [[trill consonant|trill]] and a phonemic [[glottal stop]] (represented in the orthography by 'h'). Wagiman also has a vowel inventory that is standard for the north of Australia, with a system of 5 vowels.

====Consonants====
Stops that are [[Fortis and lenis|fortis]] (or 'strong') are differentiated from those that are [[lenis]] (or 'weak') on the basis of length of closure, as opposed to the [[voice onset time]] (VOT), the period after the release of the stop before the commencement of [[Vocal folds|vocal fold]] activity (or [[Voice (phonetics)|voice]]) which normally differentiates fortis and lenis stops in English and most other languages.
Stops that are [[Fortis and lenis|fortis]] (or 'strong') are differentiated from those that are [[lenis]] (or 'weak') on the basis of length of closure, as opposed to the [[voice onset time]] (VOT), the period after the release of the stop before the commencement of [[Vocal folds|vocal fold]] activity (or [[Voice (phonetics)|voice]]) which normally differentiates fortis and lenis stops in English and most other languages.


Line 199: Line 58:
Since the length of closure is defined in terms of time between the closure of the vocal tract after the preceding vowel, and the release before the following vowel, stops at the beginning or end of a word do not have a fortis-lenis contrast. Orthographically in Wagiman, word-initial stops are written using the voiced Roman letters (''b'', ''d'' and ''g''), but at the end of a word, voiceless letters (''p'', ''t'' and ''k'') are used instead.
Since the length of closure is defined in terms of time between the closure of the vocal tract after the preceding vowel, and the release before the following vowel, stops at the beginning or end of a word do not have a fortis-lenis contrast. Orthographically in Wagiman, word-initial stops are written using the voiced Roman letters (''b'', ''d'' and ''g''), but at the end of a word, voiceless letters (''p'', ''t'' and ''k'') are used instead.


{| class="wikitable"
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align:center;"
!rowspan=2|
!rowspan=2 colspan=2|
!colspan=2| [[Peripheral consonant|Peripheral]]
!colspan=2| [[Peripheral consonant|Peripheral]]
! [[Laminal consonant|Laminal]]
! [[Laminal consonant|Laminal]]
Line 212: Line 71:
! [[retroflex consonant|Retroflex]]
! [[retroflex consonant|Retroflex]]
|-
|-
!rowspan=2| [[Plosive]]
! Lenis [[plosive consonant|stop]]
! Lenis
|'''p~b''' {{IPA|[p]}}
|'''k~g''' {{IPA|[k]}}
|'''p~b''' {{IPAblink|p}}
|'''tj~j''' {{IPA|[c]}}
|'''k~g''' {{IPAblink|k}}
|'''t~d''' {{IPA|[t]}}
|'''tj~j''' {{IPAblink|c}}
|'''rt~rd''' {{IPA|[ʈ]}}
|'''t~d''' {{IPAblink|t}}
|'''h''' {{IPA|[ʔ]}}
|'''rt~rd''' {{IPAblink|ʈ}}
|'''h''' {{IPAblink|ʔ}}
|-
|-
! Fortis stop
! Fortis
|'''pp''' {{IPA|[]}}
|'''pp''' {{IPAblink|pː}}
|'''kk''' {{IPA|[]}}
|'''kk''' {{IPAblink|kː}}
|'''ttj''' {{IPA|[]}}
|'''ttj''' {{IPAblink|cː}}
|'''tt''' {{IPA|[]}}
|'''tt''' {{IPAblink|tː}}
|'''rtt''' {{IPA|[ʈː]}}
|'''rtt''' {{IPAblink|ʈː}}
|
|
|-
|-
![[nasal stop|nasal]]
!colspan=2|[[nasal stop|Nasal]]
|'''m''' {{IPA|[m]}}
|'''m''' {{IPAblink|m}}
|'''ng''' {{IPA|[ŋ]}}
|'''ng''' {{IPAblink|ŋ}}
|'''ny''' {{IPA|[ɲ]}}
|'''ny''' {{IPAblink|ɲ}}
|'''n''' {{IPA|[n]}}
|'''n''' {{IPAblink|n}}
|'''rn''' {{IPA|[ɳ]}}
|'''rn''' {{IPAblink|ɳ}}
|
|
|-
|-
![[trill consonant|trill]]
!colspan=2|[[trill consonant|Trill]]
|
|
|
|
|
|
|'''rr''' {{IPA|[r]}}
|'''rr''' {{IPAblink|r}}
|
|
|
|
|-
|-
![[lateral consonant|lateral]]
!colspan=2|[[lateral consonant|Lateral]]
|
|
|
|
|
|
|'''l''' {{IPA|[l]}}
|'''l''' {{IPAblink|l}}
|'''rl''' {{IPA|[ɭ]}}
|'''rl''' {{IPAblink|ɭ}}
|
|
|-
|-
!colspan=2|[[Approximant]]
![[approximant consonant|approximant]]
|colspan=2|'''w''' {{IPA|[w]}}
|colspan=2|'''w''' {{IPAblink|w}}
|'''y''' {{IPA|[j]}}
|'''y''' {{IPAblink|j}}
|
|
|'''r''' {{IPA|[ɻ]}}
|'''r''' {{IPAblink|ɻ}}
|
|
|}
|}


====Vowels====
===Vowels===
As with many languages of the top-end, Wagiman has a standard five-vowel system. However, a system of [[vowel harmony]] indicates that two sets of vowels are closely associated with each other. {{IPA|[ɛ]}} aligns closely with {{IPA|[ɪ]}} and similarly, {{IPA|[ɔ]}} merges with {{IPA|[ʊ]}}.
As with many languages of the top-end, Wagiman has a standard five-vowel system. However, a system of [[vowel harmony]] indicates that two sets of vowels are closely associated with each other. {{IPA|[ɛ]}} aligns closely with {{IPA|[ɪ]}} and similarly, {{IPA|[ɔ]}} merges with {{IPA|[ʊ]}}.


In this respect, it is possible to analyse Wagiman's vowel inventory as historically deriving from a three-vowel system common among the languages from further south, but with the phonetic influence of a typically northern five-vowel system.
In this respect, it is possible to analyse Wagiman's vowel inventory as historically deriving from a three-vowel system common among the languages from further south, but with the phonetic influence of a typically northern five-vowel system.


{| class="wikitable"
{| class="wikitable" style="text-align:center;"
!
!
![[front vowel|front]]
![[front vowel|front]]
Line 272: Line 132:
|-
|-
![[close vowel|close]]
![[close vowel|close]]
|'''i''' {{IPA|[ɪ]}}
|'''i''' {{IPAblink|ɪ}}
|
|
|'''u''' {{IPA|[ʊ]}}
|'''u''' {{IPAblink|ʊ}}
|-
|-
![[mid vowel|mid]]
![[mid vowel|mid]]
|'''e''' {{IPA|[ɛ]}}
|'''e''' {{IPAblink|ɛ}}
|
|
|'''o''' {{IPA|[ɔ]}}
|'''o''' {{IPAblink|ɔ}}
|-
|-
![[open vowel|open]]
![[open vowel|open]]
|
|
|'''a''' {{IPA|[a]}}
|'''a''' {{IPAblink|a}}
|
|}
|}


====Phonotactics====
===Phonotactics===
Each [[syllable]] of Wagiman contains an [[syllable onset|onset]], a [[syllable nucleus|nucleus]] and an optional [[syllable coda|coda]]. This may be generalised to the syllable template CV(C). The coda may consist of any single consonant, a continuant and a glottal stop, or an approximant and any stop.
Each [[syllable]] of Wagiman contains an [[syllable onset|onset]], a [[syllable nucleus|nucleus]] and an optional [[syllable coda|coda]]. This may be generalised to the syllable template CV(C). The coda may consist of any single consonant, a continuant and a glottal stop, or an approximant and any stop.


At the word level, Wagiman has a [[mora (linguistics)|bimoraic]] minimum, meaning that if a word consists of a single syllable, it must have either a [[long vowel]] or a coda. Examples of monosyllabic words in Wagiman include ''yow'' {{IPA|[jɒʊ]}} 'yes', or ''jamh'' {{IPA|[ɟʌmʔ]}} 'eat.[[perfect (grammar)|{{sc|perf}}]]'.
At the word level, Wagiman has a [[mora (linguistics)|bimoraic]] minimum, meaning that if a word consists of a single syllable, it must have either a [[long vowel]] or a coda. Examples of monosyllabic words in Wagiman include {{Lang|waq|yow}} {{IPA|[jɒʊ]}} 'yes', or {{Lang|waq|jamh}} {{IPA|[ɟʌmʔ]}} 'eat.{{gcl|PERF}}'.


The retroflex approximant 'r' {{IPA|[ɻ]}} is not permitted word-initially and instead becomes a lateral 'l'. This only affects verb roots, as they are the only part of speech that takes prefixes and are therefore the only possible part of speech for which word-initial and word-medial environmental effects can be observed.
The retroflex approximant 'r' {{IPA|[ɻ]}} is not permitted word-initially and instead becomes a lateral 'l'. This only affects verb roots, as they are the only part of speech that takes prefixes and are therefore the only possible part of speech for which word-initial and word-medial environmental effects can be observed.


The verb ''ra-ndi'' 'throw', for instance, surfaces as ''la-ndi'' when inflected for third-person singular subjects (he/she/it), which are realised by invisible, or [[Null morpheme|null]] morphemes. but as ''nga-ra-ndi'' when inflected for a first-person singular subject (I). When preceded by a syllable with a coda, the 'r' similarly moves to 'l', as in ''ngan-la-ndi'' 'he/she/it threw you'. In short, the retroflex approximant 'r' {{IPA|[ɻ]}} is only realised as 'r' when it occurs between two vowels. Elsewhere, it becomes a lateral approximant 'l'.
The verb {{Lang|waq|ra-ndi}} 'throw', for instance, surfaces as {{Lang|waq|la-ndi}} when inflected for third-person singular subjects (he/she/it), which are realised by invisible, or [[Null morpheme|null]] morphemes. but as {{Lang|waq|nga-ra-ndi}} when inflected for a first-person singular subject (I). When preceded by a syllable with a coda, the 'r' similarly moves to 'l', as in ''ngan-la-ndi'' 'he/she/it threw you'. In short, the retroflex approximant 'r' {{IPA|[ɻ]}} is only realised as 'r' when it occurs between two vowels. Elsewhere, it becomes a lateral approximant 'l'.


=====Heterorganic clusters=====
====Heterorganic clusters====
Consonant clusters across syllable boundaries do not assimilate for place in Wagiman as they do in many other languages. This means that a nasal in a syllable coda will not move to the position of the following syllable onset for ease of enunciation. In English and most other [[Indo-European languages]], this movement occurs regularly, such that the prefix ''-in'', for example, changes to ''-im'' when it precedes either a ''p'', a ''b'' or an ''m''.
Consonant clusters across syllable boundaries do not assimilate for place in Wagiman as they do in many other languages. This means that a nasal in a syllable coda will not move to the position of the following syllable onset for ease of enunciation. In English and most other [[Indo-European languages]] with the exception of [[Russian language|Russian]], this movement occurs regularly, such that the prefix ''in-'', for example, changes to ''im-'' when it precedes either a ''p'', a ''b'' or an ''m''.
:''in'' + ''possible'' → ''impossible''
:''in'' + ''possible'' → ''impossible''
:''in'' + ''balance'' → ''imbalance''
:''in'' + ''balance'' → ''imbalance''
:''in'' + ''material'' → ''immaterial''
:''in'' + ''material'' → ''immaterial''
Wagiman does not do this. A nasal in a coda retains its position regardless of the following consonant:
Wagiman does not do this. A nasal in a coda retains its position regardless of the following consonant:
:''manyngardal'' 'tongue' {{IPA|[maɲŋaɖaɫ]}}
:{{Lang|waq|manyngardal}} 'tongue' {{IPA|[maɲŋaɖaɫ]}}
:''binkan'' 'bream' ''(fish spec.)'' {{IPA|[bɪnɡan]}}
:{{Lang|waq|binkan}} 'bream' ''(fish spec.)'' {{IPA|[bɪnɡan]}}
:''ngan-bu-ni'' 's/he hit me' {{IPA|[ŋanbʊnɪ]}}
:{{Lang|waq|ngan-bu-ni}} 's/he hit me' {{IPA|[ŋanbʊnɪ]}}
If Wagiman constrained against heterorganic clusters and assimilated them for place, as English does, these words would surface as {{IPA|[ma'''ŋ'''ŋaɖaɫ]}}, {{IPA|[bɪ'''ŋ'''ɡan]}}, and {{IPA|[ŋa'''m'''bʊnɪ]}}.
If Wagiman constrained against heterorganic clusters and assimilated them for place, as English does, these words would surface as {{IPA|[ma'''ŋ'''ŋaɖaɫ]}}, {{IPA|[bɪ'''ŋ'''ɡan]}}, and {{IPA|[ŋa'''m'''bʊnɪ]}}.


=====Vowel harmony=====
====Vowel harmony====
High vowels assimilate in height to following mid vowels across syllable boundaries. That is, {{IPA|[ɪ]}} will become {{IPA|[ɛ]}}, and {{IPA|[ʊ]}} will become {{IPA|[ɔ]}}, when the following syllable contains a mid vowel; either {{IPA|[ɛ]}} or {{IPA|[ɔ]}}.
High vowels assimilate in height to following mid vowels across syllable boundaries. That is, {{IPA|[ɪ]}} will become {{IPA|[ɛ]}}, and {{IPA|[ʊ]}} will become {{IPA|[ɔ]}}, when the following syllable contains a mid vowel; either {{IPA|[ɛ]}} or {{IPA|[ɔ]}}.


{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2
:''mi-'' (2sg.[[imperative mood|{{sc|imp}}]]) and ''-ge'' ('put'), becomes ''mege'' 'you go and put it'.
|mi- + -ge → mege
:''mu-'' (2pl.[[imperative mood|{{sc|imp}}]]) and ''-yobe'' ('stay'), becomes ''moyobe'' 'you lot stay'.
|2SG.IMP {} 'put' {} {'you go and put it'}|}}

{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2
|mu- + -yobe → moyobe
|2PL.IMP {} 'stay' {} {'you lot stay'}|}}


Wagiman vowel harmony and other aspects of Wagiman phonotactics require further investigation. It is not known, for instance, whether vowel harmony equally affects unstressed syllables.
Wagiman vowel harmony and other aspects of Wagiman phonotactics require further investigation. It is not known, for instance, whether vowel harmony equally affects unstressed syllables.

==Grammar==
''All grammatical information from Wilson, S. (1999)''<ref name="Wilson, S. 1999"/> ''unless otherwise noted.''

===Parts of speech===
The three most important parts of speech in Wagiman are [[verb]]s, [[coverb]]s and [[nominal (linguistics)|nominals]]. Apart from these, there are a multitude of verbal and nominal [[affix]]es, [[interjection]]s and other particles. [[Pronoun]]s class with nominals.

====Nominals====
Like many Australian languages, Wagiman does not categorically distinguish [[noun]]s from [[adjective]]s. These form one word class that is called nominals. Wagiman nominals take case suffixes (see [[#Nominals 2|below]]) that denote their grammatical or semantic role in the sentence. The grammatical cases are ergative and absolutive, and the semantic cases include instrumental (using), allative (towards), ablative (from), locative (at), comitative (with, having), privative (without, lacking), temporal (at the time of) and semblative (resembling). The dative case can be either grammatical or semantic, depending on the syntactic requirements of the verb.

[[Demonstratives]] are similarly considered nominals in Wagiman, and take the same case suffixes depending on their semantic and syntactic roles; their function within the sentence. That is, the demonstrative {{Lang|waq|mahan}} 'this', or 'here' (root: {{Lang|waq|mayh-}}), may take case just like any other nominal.
*{{Lang|waq|mayh-yi}} this-{{gcl|ERG}} 'this one (did it)'
*{{Lang|waq|mayh-ga}} this-{{gcl|ALL}} 'to here'

=====Examples of nominals=====
*{{Lang|waq|guda}} 'fire', 'wood' {{IPA|[ɡʊda]}}
*{{Lang|waq|wirin}} 'tree', 'stick' {{IPA|[wɪɻɪn]}}
*{{Lang|waq|lagiban}} 'man' {{IPA|[laɡɪban]}}
*{{Lang|waq|gordal}} 'head' {{IPA|[ɡɔɖaɫ]}}
*{{Lang|waq|lagiriny}} 'tail' {{IPA|[laɡɪɻɪɲ]}}
*{{Lang|waq|manyngardal}} 'tongue' {{IPA|[maɲŋaɖaɫ]}}

====Pronouns====
Pronouns are typologically nominals also, yet their [[morphosyntactic alignment]] is [[nominative–accusative language|nominative–accusative]] rather than ergative–absolutive.

{| class="wikitable"
! colspan="3" |
![[Nominative case|Nominative]]
![[Accusative case|Accusative]]
![[Genitive case|Genitive]]
|-
! rowspan="4" | 1st<br> person
! colspan="2" | singular
|{{lang|waq|ngagun}}
|{{lang|waq|nganung}}
|{{lang|waq|nganing-gin}}
|-
! colspan="2" | dual
|{{lang|waq|nginyang}}
|{{lang|waq|nginyang}}
|{{lang|waq|nginyang-gin}}
|-
! rowspan="2" | plural
! {{gcl|EXCL|glossing=wikilink}}
|{{lang|waq|ngego}}
|{{lang|waq|ngerreju}}
|{{lang|waq|gerdo-gin}}
|-
! {{gcl|INCL|glossing=wikilink}}
|{{lang|waq|ngego}}
|{{lang|waq|ngerre-ngana}}
|{{lang|waq|gerdo-gin}}
|-
! rowspan="2" | 2nd<br> person
! colspan="2" | singular
|{{lang|waq|ngigun}}
|{{lang|waq|ngonggo}}
|{{lang|waq|ngonggo-gin}}
|-
! colspan="2" | plural
|{{lang|waq|ngogo}}
|{{lang|waq|ngorruju}}
|{{lang|waq|gordo-gin}}
|-
! rowspan="2" | 3rd<br> person
! colspan="2" | singular
|{{lang|waq|ngonggega}} (rare)
|{{lang|waq|nung}}
|{{lang|waq|nung-gin}}
|-
! colspan="2" | plural
|{{lang|waq|bogo}} (rare)
|{{lang|waq|borruju}}
|{{lang|waq|borro-gin}}
|}

The 3rd person singular and plural nominative forms, {{Lang|waq|ngonggega}} and {{Lang|waq|bogo}}, are labeled 'rare' because they are gradually becoming disused. Speakers prefer to use non-personal pronouns such as {{Lang|waq|gayh}}''-'' 'that' or {{Lang|waq|gayh-gorden}} 'those'. Moreover, since the person and number of the subject is contained in the prefix of the verb, nominative free pronouns are often dropped.

=====Tripartite alignment=====
[[Image:Ergative alignment.svg|thumb|right|250px|Ergative alignment of the case system]]
While the nominal case system distinguishes the ergative case from absolutive, the free pronouns distinguish nominative from accusative, as shown above. However, they inflect for ergative case as well, resulting in a [[Tripartite language|tripartite]] case system, as in the following:

{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|ngagun-yi ngonggo ngany-bu-ng
|1SG.NOM-ERG 2SG.ACC {{gcl|1sgA|1st-person singular agent}}.{{gcl|2sgO|second-person singular object}}-hit-PFV
|'I hit you.'
}}

[[Image:Accusative alignment.svg|thumb|right|250px|Accusative alignment of the free pronoun system. <br />A = Transitive agent <br />S = Intransitive argument <br /> O = Transitive object]]
The nominative pronoun root in this instance, {{Lang|waq|ngagun}} 'I', takes the ergative case suffix ''-yi'' to denote the fact that it is the agent of a transitive clause. Conversely, the same pronoun does not take the ergative case when acting as the argument of an intransitive clause:

{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|ngagun maman nga-yu
|1SG.NOM good 1SG-be.PRS
|'I am good.'
}}

The accusative pronouns on the other hand, may be accusative or dative, depending on the syntactic requirements of the verb. In the traditional terminology, these pronouns can be either direct or indirect objects.

{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|ngagun-yi nga-nanda-yi nung
|1SG.NOM-ERG {{gcl|1sgA|1st-person singular "agent"}}.{{gcl|3sgO|3rd-person singular "object"}}-see-PST 3SG.ACC
|'I saw him/her.'
}}

{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|nga-nawu-ndi wahan nung
|{{gcl|1sgA|1st-person singular agent}}.{{gcl|3sgO|3rd-person singular object}}-give-PST water 3SG.ACC
|'I gave the water to him/her.'
}}

For these reasons, the pronouns are also labeled ''base'' for nominative–ergative pronouns, and ''oblique'' for accusative–dative pronouns.

=====Genitive pronouns=====
In the table above, genitive pronouns all end with ''-gin'', which is separated orthographically by a hyphen that normally divides morphemes. The ''-gin'' form here is not a separate morpheme and cannot be lexically segmented; there is no such word as {{Lang|waq|nganing}} that would be formed by removing ''-gin'' from {{Lang|waq|nganing-gin}} 'my/mine'. The fact that the genitive forms have regular endings across the entire pronoun paradigm may have been a historical accident.

This cannot be a nominal suffix like those listed above, since it may not attach to other nominals ({{Lang|waq|*warren-gin lari}} 'the child's hand', but {{Lang|waq|warren-gu lari}} 'the child's hand'). Furthermore, the genitive pronouns may take a further case suffix, as in the example:

{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|goron nganing-gin-ba
|house my-ALL
|'To my house.'
}}

This would be prohibited by the restriction against [[case stacking]] in Wagiman if the genitive ''-gin'' were a case suffix.

====Verbs====
Verbs are a class of word in Wagiman which contains fewer than 50 members. As it is a closed class, no more verbs are possible. They are often monosyllabic verb roots and all are vowel-final. Wagiman verbs obligatorily inflect for person and number of core arguments, and for the tense and aspect of the clause. A small set of verbs may take a non-finite suffix ''-yh'', in which it may not be further inflected for person or tense. That non-finite verb must then co-occur with another [[auxiliary verb]].

=====Examples of verbs=====
Each verb is listed with its past tense marker, which is the second morpheme. Pronunciation given where appropriate.
*{{Lang|waq|bu-ni}} 'hit'
*{{Lang|waq|di-nya}} 'come'
*{{Lang|waq|la-ndi}} 'throw'
*{{Lang|waq|rinyi-ra}} 'fall'
*{{Lang|waq|nanda-yi}} 'see' {{IPA|[nandaɪ]}}
*{{Lang|waq|yu-nginy}} 'be' {{IPA|[jʊŋɪɲ]}}

====Coverbs====
There are so far over 500 recorded coverbs in Wagiman, and more are discovered with continuing research. Compared with verbs, coverbs are far more numerous and far more semantically rich. Verbs express simple, broad meanings such as ''yu-'' 'be', ''ya-'' 'go' and ''di-'' 'come', while coverbs convey more specific, semantically narrow meanings such as {{Lang|waq|barnhbarn-na}} 'make footprints', {{Lang|waq|lerdongh-nga}} 'play (a [[didgeridoo]])' or {{Lang|waq|murr-ma}} 'wade through shallow water using your feet to search for something'.

Coverbs however, cannot inflect for person and cannot, in themselves, head finite clauses. If they are to act as the head of a clause, they must combine with a verb, thereby forming a bipartite verbal compound, commonly called a [[#Complex predicates|complex predicate]].

=====Examples of coverbs=====
Each is listed with the ''-ma'' suffix (or its allomorph), which signals aspectual unmarkedness.
*{{Lang|waq|liri-ma}} 'swim' {{IPA|[lɪɻɪma]}}
*{{Lang|waq|dabaley-ma}} '(go) around)' {{IPA|[dabalema]}}
*{{Lang|waq|gorrh-ma}} 'fish' {{IPA|[ɡɔrʔma]}}
*{{Lang|waq|dippart-ta}} 'jump' {{IPA|[dɪbˑaɖˑa]}}
*{{Lang|waq|wirrnh-na}} 'whistle' {{IPA|[wɪrʔna]}}


===Syntax===
===Syntax===
Line 325: Line 341:
Further to these affixes, verbs may be marked for the number of the subject, be it dual or plural, and also for [[clusivity]]; whether the listener is included in the described event (inclusive) or is excluded from the event (exclusive).
Further to these affixes, verbs may be marked for the number of the subject, be it dual or plural, and also for [[clusivity]]; whether the listener is included in the described event (inclusive) or is excluded from the event (exclusive).


Verb morphology in Wagiman is highly irregular. Of the small inventory of inflecting verbs, many have their own unique tense suffixes, while other tense suffixes are common to several verbs, and while some rudimentary verb classes can be identified - stance verbs always take the past tense suffix ''-nginy'' {{IPA|/ŋɪɲ/}}, for instance - the tense suffixes must be learned for each individual verb.
The verbal morphology of tense suffixes in Wagiman is irregular. Of the small inventory of inflecting verbs, many have their own unique tense suffixes, while other tense suffixes are common to several verbs, and while some rudimentary verb classes can be identified - stance verbs always take the past tense suffix ''-nginy'' {{IPA|/ŋɪɲ/}}, for instance - the tense suffixes must be learned for each individual verb.


The prefixes on the other hand, are regular for each verb, although the complete paradigm of verb prefixes is highly complex. They encode three variables: person, number and tense, and are not segmentable; one prefix cannot be separated into the three parts. ''Ngani-'' for example, encodes second-person singular [[Agent (grammar)|agent]] ('you'), first-person singular [[Patient (grammar)|patient]]/undergoer ('me') as well as past tense.
The prefixes on the other hand, are regular for each verb, although the complete paradigm of verb prefixes is highly complex. They encode three variables: person, number and tense, and are only segmentable in a few cases; one prefix cannot be separated into the three parts. ''Ngani-'' for example, encodes second-person singular [[Agent (grammar)|agent]] ('you'), first-person singular [[Patient (grammar)|patient]]/undergoer ('me') as well as past tense.


:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|ngani-bu-ng
|ngani-bu-ng
|{{gcl|2sgA|2nd-person singular agent}}.{{gcl|1sgO|1st-person singular object}}.PST-hit-PFV
|{{gcl|2sgA|2nd-person singular agent}}.{{gcl|1sgO|1st-person singular object}}.PST-hit-PFV
|'You hit me.'}}
|'You hit me.'
}}


=====Nominals=====
=====Nominals=====
Line 338: Line 355:


Apart from the grammatical cases, ergative and absolutive, which are necessary to construct meaningful sentences, an entire range of semantic cases occur with very high frequency, even when their meaning can be expressed without using case. In the following examples, the former, in which no case is used, is far less common than the latter:
Apart from the grammatical cases, ergative and absolutive, which are necessary to construct meaningful sentences, an entire range of semantic cases occur with very high frequency, even when their meaning can be expressed without using case. In the following examples, the former, in which no case is used, is far less common than the latter:

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|wuji nga-nga-gondo-n garradin
|wuji nga-nga-gondo-n garradin
|NEG IRR-1SG-have-PRS money
|NEG IRR-1SG-have-PRS money
|'I don't have any money.'}}
|'I don't have any money.'
}}


:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|garrad-'''nehen''' nga-yu
|garrad-'''nehen''' nga-yu
|money-'''PRIV''' 1SG.PRS-be
|money-'''PRIV''' 1SG.PRS-be
|'I am without money.' or 'I am penniless.'}}
|'I am without money.' or 'I am penniless.'
}}

There are also some bound particles, which appear to function in much the same syntactic manner as cases, but which are not considered 'case', for theoretical reasons. -''binyju'' {{IPA|/bɪɲɟʊ/}} 'only' is one of these nominal particles, as in:


{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
There are also some bound particles, which appear to function in much the same syntactic manner as cases, but which are not considered 'case', for theoretical reasons. ''-Binyju'' {{IPA|/bɪɲɟʊ/}} 'only' is one of these nominal particles, as in:
:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
|gubiji-'''binyju''' bula-ndi
|gubiji-'''binyju''' bula-ndi
|bone-'''only''' 3SG.leave-PST
|bone-'''only''' 3SG.leave-PST
|'S/he left only the bones.'}}
|'S/he left only the bones.'
}}


Nominals are also marked for number with a suffix that adjoins directly to the root, inside the case suffix. ''-giwu'' 'two', for example, would attach to the nominal root before the case, as in:
Nominals are also marked for number with a suffix that adjoins directly to the root, inside the case suffix. ''-giwu'' 'two', for example, would attach to the nominal root before the case, as in:

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|lamarra-giwu-yi nganba-badi-na
|lamarra-giwu-yi nganba-badi-na
|dog-two-ERG {{gcl|3plA|3rd-person plural agent}}.{{gcl|1sgO|1st-person singular object}}-bite-PST
|dog-two-ERG {{gcl|3plA|3rd-person plural agent}}.{{gcl|1sgO|1st-person singular object}}-bite-PST
|'the two dogs bit me.'}}
|'the two dogs bit me.'
}}


As cases cannot be [[case stacking|stacked]] in Wagiman, these number suffixes cannot be called case suffixes, whereas the nominal suffixes discussed above (such as ''-binyju'' 'only'), show the same syntactic distribution - they occur in the same place - and therefore may be analysed as cases themselves.
As cases cannot be [[case stacking|stacked]] in Wagiman, these number suffixes cannot be called case suffixes, whereas the nominal suffixes discussed above (such as ''-binyju'' 'only'), show the same syntactic distribution - they occur in the same place - and therefore may be analysed as cases themselves.
Line 365: Line 389:
Coverbs also have their own set of inflectional morphemes, such as aspect, but may also take semantic case suffixes (all those listed above except for ergative and absolutive). For instance, a coverb may take the dative case to convey intention, or purpose, as in:
Coverbs also have their own set of inflectional morphemes, such as aspect, but may also take semantic case suffixes (all those listed above except for ergative and absolutive). For instance, a coverb may take the dative case to convey intention, or purpose, as in:


:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|liri-ma-gu
|liri-ma-gu
|swim-ASP-DAT
|swim-ASP-DAT
|'for swimming'}}
|'for swimming'
}}


Coverbs are categorially differentiated from nominals though, in that a nominal may not take the aspectual suffixes that a coverb obligatorily takes.
Coverbs are categorially differentiated from nominals though, in that a nominal may not take the aspectual suffixes that a coverb obligatorily takes.
Line 375: Line 400:


The ''-ma'' suffix exhibits regular [[allomorphy]]; it assimilates in [[place of articulation|place]] and [[manner of articulation]] to any preceding [[obstruent]] or [[nasal stop|nasal]], but not to any preceding [[lateral consonant|lateral]], [[rhotic consonant|rhotic]] or [[approximant consonant|approximant]]. That is, it remains ''-ma'' following vowels, or following the consonants [r], [l], [w] and [j], but when it follows [p], for instance, it assimilates in manner and place, and becomes /-pa/, as in ''dup-pa'' 'sit'.
The ''-ma'' suffix exhibits regular [[allomorphy]]; it assimilates in [[place of articulation|place]] and [[manner of articulation]] to any preceding [[obstruent]] or [[nasal stop|nasal]], but not to any preceding [[lateral consonant|lateral]], [[rhotic consonant|rhotic]] or [[approximant consonant|approximant]]. That is, it remains ''-ma'' following vowels, or following the consonants [r], [l], [w] and [j], but when it follows [p], for instance, it assimilates in manner and place, and becomes /-pa/, as in ''dup-pa'' 'sit'.
*''liri'' + ''ma'' → ''liri-ma''
*''liri'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|liri-ma}}
*''wal'' + ''ma'' → ''wal-ma''
*''wal'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|wal-ma}}
*''bey'' + ''ma'' → ''bey-ma''
*''bey'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|bey-ma}}
*''yorony'' + ''ma'' → ''yorony-nya''
*''yorony'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|yorony-nya}}
*''datj'' + ''ma'' → ''datj-ja''
*''datj'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|datj-ja}}
The inclusion of the glottal stop in certain words, is ineffective to the surface realisation of the ''-ma'' suffix; it will change, or remain unchanged, according to whichever segment precedes the glottal stop, as in:
The inclusion of the glottal stop in certain words, is ineffective to the surface realisation of the ''-ma'' suffix; it will change, or remain unchanged, according to whichever segment precedes the glottal stop, as in:
*''wunh'' + ''ma'' → ''wunh-na''
*''wunh'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|wunh-na}}
*''gayh'' + ''ma'' → ''gayh-ma''
*''gayh'' + ''ma'' → {{Lang|waq|gayh-ma}}
Cross-linguistically, the''-ma'' suffix may be related to a coverbial suffix in Jaminjung, a language in which coverb roots occur without any aspect markers, but are then suffixed with ''-mayan'', which marks continuous aspect. This coverb suffix bears a striking resemblance to the sum of the Wagiman ''-ma'' suffix and the continuous aspect suffix ''-yan'', which always occur in tandem on coverbs. Together, ''-ma'' and ''-yan'' perform the same semantic function as Jaminjung ''-mayan''. Precisely what the relationship holds between these suffixes; whether one language borrowed from the other, or whether each language inherited them from earlier languages, is not at all clear.
Cross-linguistically, the''-ma'' suffix may be related to a coverbial suffix in Jaminjung, a language in which coverb roots occur without any aspect markers, but are then suffixed with ''-mayan'', which marks continuous aspect. This coverb suffix bears a striking resemblance to the sum of the Wagiman ''-ma'' suffix and the continuous aspect suffix ''-yan'', which always occur in tandem on coverbs. Together, ''-ma'' and ''-yan'' perform the same semantic function as Jaminjung ''-mayan''. Precisely what the relationship holds between these suffixes; whether one language borrowed from the other, or whether each language inherited them from earlier languages, is not at all clear.


====Reduplication====
====Reduplication====
Further to derivational and inflectional morphemes, Wagiman coverbs and nominals often undergo [[reduplication]], whereby a part, or often the entirety of the root, is repeated. Reduplication can convey a multitude of meanings. When coverbs are reduplicated, the resulting derived coverb may involve added meaning components such as iterativity, duration or habituality.
Further to derivational and inflectional morphemes, Wagiman coverbs and nominals often undergo [[reduplication]], whereby a part, or often the entirety of the root, is repeated. Reduplication can convey a multitude of meanings. When coverbs are reduplicated, the resulting derived coverb may involve added meaning components such as iterativity, duration or habituality.

:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|dabulp-pa ga-ya nu-naw-ma
|dabulp-pa ga-ya nu-naw-ma
|smoke-ASP 3SG.PRS.go lots
|smoke-ASP 3SG.PRS.go lots
|'S/he smokes lots.'}}
|'S/he smokes lots.'
}}


:{{interlinear|glossing = wikilink|ablist = RDP:reduplication:Reduplication
{{interlinear |lang=waq |indent=2 |glossing=wikilink
|dabuldabulp ga-ya
|dabuldabulp ga-ya
|smoke.RDP 3SG.PRS.go
|smoke.REDUP 3SG.PRS.go
|'S/he smokes all the time.'}}
|'S/he smokes all the time.'
}}


When nominals are derived by reduplication, the added meaning is usually one of plurality. However, since both a dual and a plural nominal suffix exist, ''-giwu'' and ''-guju'' respectively, nominal reduplication is rare.
When nominals are derived by reduplication, the added meaning is usually one of plurality. However, since both a dual and a plural nominal suffix exist, ''-giwu'' and ''-guju'' respectively, nominal reduplication is rare.
Line 401: Line 429:
====Complex predicates====
====Complex predicates====
{{main|Complex predicate}}
{{main|Complex predicate}}
A complex predicate is the combination of more than one element, more than one individual word, to convey the information involved in a single event.<ref>Butt, M. (2003: 2)</ref> For instance, the event ''swim'' is conveyed in Wagiman using a combination of a verb ''ya-'' 'go' and a coverb ''liri-ma'' 'swimming'. There is no verb in Wagiman that, on its own, conveys the event of swimming.
A complex predicate is the combination of more than one element, more than one individual word, to convey the information involved in a single event.<ref>Butt, M. (2003: 2)</ref> For instance, the event ''swim'' is conveyed in Wagiman using a combination of a verb ''ya-'' 'go' and a coverb {{Lang|waq|liri-ma}} 'swimming'. There is no verb in Wagiman that, on its own, conveys the event of swimming.


Bipartite verbal compounds such as these are not peculiar to any language in particular. They are in fact very common, and may even occur in every language, albeit with varying frequency. English has a number of complex predicates, include ''go sightseeing'', ''have breakfast'' and ''take (a) bath''. The event described by ''go sightseeing'' is unable to be described using a single verb ''sightsee''; inflections like ''sightsaw'' and ''sightseen'' are ungrammatical. An event like ''take (a) bath'', however, may be described by a single verb ''bathe'', but it arguably has a slightly different meaning. ''Take (a) bath'', in any case, is far more common.
Bipartite verbal compounds such as these are not peculiar to any language in particular. They are in fact very common, and may even occur in every language, albeit with varying frequency. English has a number of complex predicates, include ''go sightseeing'', ''have breakfast'' and ''take (a) bath''. The event described by ''go sightseeing'' is unable to be described using a single verb ''sightsee''; inflections like ''sightsaw'' and ''sightseen'' are ungrammatical. An event like ''take (a) bath'', however, may be described by a single verb ''bathe'', but it arguably has a slightly different meaning. ''Take (a) bath'', in any case, is far more common.
Line 426: Line 454:
*Cook, Anthony R. [http://www1.aiatsis.gov.au/ASEDA/eMU_W.htm#Wagiman ''Wagiman Matyin: a description of the Wagiman language of the Northern Territory.''] PhD Thesis. Melbourne: La Trobe University, 1987.
*Cook, Anthony R. [http://www1.aiatsis.gov.au/ASEDA/eMU_W.htm#Wagiman ''Wagiman Matyin: a description of the Wagiman language of the Northern Territory.''] PhD Thesis. Melbourne: La Trobe University, 1987.
*Evans, Nicholas. ''Bininj Gun-Wok: A pan-dialectal grammar of Mayali, Kunwinjku and Kune.'' Volumes 1 and 2. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. 2003. {{ISBN|0-85883-530-4}}
*Evans, Nicholas. ''Bininj Gun-Wok: A pan-dialectal grammar of Mayali, Kunwinjku and Kune.'' Volumes 1 and 2. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics. 2003. {{ISBN|0-85883-530-4}}
*Harvey, Mark. ''Western Gunwinyguan''. In Nicholas Evans, ed. ''The non-Pama–Nyungan languages of northern Australia: comparative studies of the continent's most linguistically complex region'', 285-303. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics, 2003. {{ISBN|0-85883-538-X}}
*Harvey, Mark. ''Western Gunwinyguan''. In Nicholas Evans, ed. ''The non-Pama–Nyungan languages of northern Australia: comparative studies of the continent's most linguistically complex region'', 285–303. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics, 2003. {{ISBN|0-85883-538-X}}
*Merlan, Francesa C. ''A Grammar of Wardaman: A Language of the Northern Territory of Australia.'' Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 1994. {{ISBN|3-11-012942-6}}
*Merlan, Francesa C. ''A Grammar of Wardaman: A Language of the Northern Territory of Australia.'' Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 1994. {{ISBN|3-11-012942-6}}
*Wilson, Aidan. ''[http://hdl.handle.net/2123/5385 Negative evidence in linguistics: The case of Wagiman complex predicates.]'' The University of Sydney, 2006.
*Wilson, Aidan. ''[http://hdl.handle.net/2123/5385 Negative evidence in linguistics: The case of Wagiman complex predicates.]'' The University of Sydney, 2006.
Line 432: Line 460:
*Wilson, Stephen. ''[http://www1.aiatsis.gov.au/exhibitions/e_access/digital/a339234/langspeakers.html The language and its speakers]'' Wagiman on-line dictionary. Canberra: AIATSIS, 2001.
*Wilson, Stephen. ''[http://www1.aiatsis.gov.au/exhibitions/e_access/digital/a339234/langspeakers.html The language and its speakers]'' Wagiman on-line dictionary. Canberra: AIATSIS, 2001.


{{Australian Aboriginal languages}}
{{Languages of Australia}}
{{language families}}
{{language families}}
{{good article}}


[[Category:Language isolates of Australia]]
[[Category:Language isolates of Australia]]

Revision as of 04:05, 16 March 2024

Wagiman
Wageman
RegionPine Creek, Northern Territory, Australia
EthnicityWagiman
Native speakers
11 (2005)[1]
Language codes
ISO 639-3waq
Glottologwage1238
AIATSIS[3]N27
ELPWagiman
Wagiman (purple), among other non-Pama-Nyungan languages (grey)
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Wagiman, also spelt Wageman, Wakiman, Wogeman,[4] and other variants, is a near-extinct Aboriginal Australian language spoken by a small number of Wagiman people[5][3] in and around Pine Creek, in the Katherine Region of the Northern Territory.

The Wagiman language is notable within linguistics for its complex system of verbal morphology, which remains under-investigated, its possession of a cross-linguistically rare part of speech called a coverb, its complex predicates and for its ability to productively verbalise coverbs.

As of 1999 Wagiman was expected to become extinct within the next generation, as the youngest generation spoke no Wagiman and understood very little.[6] The 2011 Australian census recorded 30 speakers, while the 2016 Australian census recorded 18 speakers.[3]

Language and speakers

Relation with other languages

Jaben; the Frilled-Neck Lizard, one of the Wagiman people's sacred sites

Wagiman is a language isolate within the hypothetical Australian language family.[2] It was once assumed to be a member of the adjacent Gunwinyguan family that stretches from Arnhem Land, throughout Kakadu National Park and south to Katherine,[7] but this has since been rejected. Wagiman may still bear a remote relation with its neighbouring languages but this is yet to be demonstrated.[8]

Francesca Merlan believes that Wagiman may be distantly related to the Yangmanic languages, citing that they both use verbal particles in a similar way, to the exclusion of neighbouring languages (such as Jawoyn and Mangarrayi).[7] Stephen Wilson additionally notes some other similarities, such as in the pronominal prefixes and the marking of non-case-marked nominals. However both languages have a very low cognacy rate (shared vocabulary) of about 10%. Wagiman is also superficially similar to the neighbouring Gunwinyguan languages phonologically (both share a fortis/lenis stop contrast and a phonemic glottal stop) and to the Mirndi language Jaminjung-Ngaliwurru in the use of coverbs.[9] Mark Harvey notes similarities in the verbal inflectional systems between Wagiman and the neighbouring Eastern Daly languages.[10]

Speakers

Wagiman is the ancestral language of the Wagiman people, Aboriginal Australians whose traditional land, before colonisation, extended for hundreds of square kilometres from the Stuart Highway, throughout the Mid-Daly Basin, and across the Daly River.[11] The land is highly fertile and well-watered, and contains a number of cattle stations, on which many members of the ethnic group used to work. These stations include Claravale, Dorisvale, Jindare, Oolloo and Douglas.[12]

The language region borders Waray to the north, Mayali (Kunwinjku) and Jawoyn on the east, Wardaman and Jaminjung on the south, and Murrinh-Patha, Ngan'giwumirri and Malak Malak on the west. Before colonisation, the lands surrounding Pine Creek, extending north to Brock's Creek, were traditionally associated with another language group that is now extinct, believed to have been Wulwulam.[13]

The dominant language of the region is Mayali, a dialect of Bininj Kunwok traditionally associated with the region surrounding Maningrida, in Western Arnhem Land.[14] As it is a strong language with hundreds of speakers and a high rate of child acquisition, members of the Wagiman ethnic group gradually ceased teaching the Wagiman language to their children. As a result, many Wagiman people speak Mayali, while only a handful of elders continue to speak Wagiman.[citation needed]

In 1987 it was found that adults in the community understood the Wagiman language to a certain extent or knew only a few basic words, but speak Daly River Kriol as their daily language. The youngest generation understood very little Wagiman and spoke none.[15] As of 1999 Wagiman was expected to become extinct within the next generation, as the youngest generation spoke no Wagiman and understood very little.[6] In 2005 only 10 speakers were recorded,[5] but the 2011 Australian census recorded 30 speakers, with the 2016 Australian census recording 18 speakers.[3]

Apart from Mayali, Kriol, a creole language based on the vocabulary of English, is the lingua franca of the area.[5] The Wagiman people are also partial speakers of a number of other languages, including Jaminjung, Wardaman and Dagoman.[citation needed]

Dialects

Wagiman speakers are conscious of a distinction between two dialects of Wagiman, which they refer to as matjjin norohma 'light language' and matjjin gunawutjjan 'heavy language'. The differences are minor and speakers have no difficulty understanding one another.[16]

Phonology and orthography

The Wagiman phonemic inventory is quite typical for a northern Australian language. It has six places of articulation with a stop and a nasal in each. There are also a number of laterals and approximants, a trill and a phonemic glottal stop (represented in the orthography by 'h'). Wagiman also has a vowel inventory that is standard for the north of Australia, with a system of five vowels.

Consonants

Stops that are fortis (or 'strong') are differentiated from those that are lenis (or 'weak') on the basis of length of closure, as opposed to the voice onset time (VOT), the period after the release of the stop before the commencement of vocal fold activity (or voice) which normally differentiates fortis and lenis stops in English and most other languages.

Lenis stops in Wagiman sound like English voiced stops and are therefore written using the Roman alphabet letters b, d and g. Fortis stops, however, sound more like voiceless stops in English, but are slightly longer than lenis stops. They are written with two voiceless letters, pp, tt and kk when they occur between two vowels.

Since the length of closure is defined in terms of time between the closure of the vocal tract after the preceding vowel, and the release before the following vowel, stops at the beginning or end of a word do not have a fortis-lenis contrast. Orthographically in Wagiman, word-initial stops are written using the voiced Roman letters (b, d and g), but at the end of a word, voiceless letters (p, t and k) are used instead.

Peripheral Laminal Apical Glottal
Bilabial Velar Palatal Alveolar Retroflex
Plosive Lenis p~b [p] k~g [k] tj~j [c] t~d [t] rt~rd [ʈ] h [ʔ]
Fortis pp [] kk [] ttj [] tt [] rtt [ʈː]
Nasal m [m] ng [ŋ] ny [ɲ] n [n] rn [ɳ]
Trill rr [r]
Lateral l [l] rl [ɭ]
Approximant w [w] y [j] r [ɻ]

Vowels

As with many languages of the top-end, Wagiman has a standard five-vowel system. However, a system of vowel harmony indicates that two sets of vowels are closely associated with each other. [ɛ] aligns closely with [ɪ] and similarly, [ɔ] merges with [ʊ].

In this respect, it is possible to analyse Wagiman's vowel inventory as historically deriving from a three-vowel system common among the languages from further south, but with the phonetic influence of a typically northern five-vowel system.

front central back
close i [ɪ] u [ʊ]
mid e [ɛ] o [ɔ]
open a [a]

Phonotactics

Each syllable of Wagiman contains an onset, a nucleus and an optional coda. This may be generalised to the syllable template CV(C). The coda may consist of any single consonant, a continuant and a glottal stop, or an approximant and any stop.

At the word level, Wagiman has a bimoraic minimum, meaning that if a word consists of a single syllable, it must have either a long vowel or a coda. Examples of monosyllabic words in Wagiman include yow [jɒʊ] 'yes', or jamh [ɟʌmʔ] 'eat.PERF'.

The retroflex approximant 'r' [ɻ] is not permitted word-initially and instead becomes a lateral 'l'. This only affects verb roots, as they are the only part of speech that takes prefixes and are therefore the only possible part of speech for which word-initial and word-medial environmental effects can be observed.

The verb ra-ndi 'throw', for instance, surfaces as la-ndi when inflected for third-person singular subjects (he/she/it), which are realised by invisible, or null morphemes. but as nga-ra-ndi when inflected for a first-person singular subject (I). When preceded by a syllable with a coda, the 'r' similarly moves to 'l', as in ngan-la-ndi 'he/she/it threw you'. In short, the retroflex approximant 'r' [ɻ] is only realised as 'r' when it occurs between two vowels. Elsewhere, it becomes a lateral approximant 'l'.

Heterorganic clusters

Consonant clusters across syllable boundaries do not assimilate for place in Wagiman as they do in many other languages. This means that a nasal in a syllable coda will not move to the position of the following syllable onset for ease of enunciation. In English and most other Indo-European languages with the exception of Russian, this movement occurs regularly, such that the prefix in-, for example, changes to im- when it precedes either a p, a b or an m.

in + possibleimpossible
in + balanceimbalance
in + materialimmaterial

Wagiman does not do this. A nasal in a coda retains its position regardless of the following consonant:

manyngardal 'tongue' [maɲŋaɖaɫ]
binkan 'bream' (fish spec.) [bɪnɡan]
ngan-bu-ni 's/he hit me' [ŋanbʊnɪ]

If Wagiman constrained against heterorganic clusters and assimilated them for place, as English does, these words would surface as [maŋŋaɖaɫ], [bɪŋɡan], and [ŋambʊnɪ].

Vowel harmony

High vowels assimilate in height to following mid vowels across syllable boundaries. That is, [ɪ] will become [ɛ], and [ʊ] will become [ɔ], when the following syllable contains a mid vowel; either [ɛ] or [ɔ].

mi-

2SG.IMP

+

 

-ge

'put'

 

mege

'you go and put it'

mi- + -ge → mege

2SG.IMP {} 'put' {} {'you go and put it'}

mu-

2PL.IMP

+

 

-yobe

'stay'

 

moyobe

'you lot stay'

mu- + -yobe → moyobe

2PL.IMP {} 'stay' {} {'you lot stay'}

Wagiman vowel harmony and other aspects of Wagiman phonotactics require further investigation. It is not known, for instance, whether vowel harmony equally affects unstressed syllables.

Grammar

All grammatical information from Wilson, S. (1999)[6] unless otherwise noted.

Parts of speech

The three most important parts of speech in Wagiman are verbs, coverbs and nominals. Apart from these, there are a multitude of verbal and nominal affixes, interjections and other particles. Pronouns class with nominals.

Nominals

Like many Australian languages, Wagiman does not categorically distinguish nouns from adjectives. These form one word class that is called nominals. Wagiman nominals take case suffixes (see below) that denote their grammatical or semantic role in the sentence. The grammatical cases are ergative and absolutive, and the semantic cases include instrumental (using), allative (towards), ablative (from), locative (at), comitative (with, having), privative (without, lacking), temporal (at the time of) and semblative (resembling). The dative case can be either grammatical or semantic, depending on the syntactic requirements of the verb.

Demonstratives are similarly considered nominals in Wagiman, and take the same case suffixes depending on their semantic and syntactic roles; their function within the sentence. That is, the demonstrative mahan 'this', or 'here' (root: mayh-), may take case just like any other nominal.

  • mayh-yi this-ERG 'this one (did it)'
  • mayh-ga this-ALL 'to here'
Examples of nominals
  • guda 'fire', 'wood' [ɡʊda]
  • wirin 'tree', 'stick' [wɪɻɪn]
  • lagiban 'man' [laɡɪban]
  • gordal 'head' [ɡɔɖaɫ]
  • lagiriny 'tail' [laɡɪɻɪɲ]
  • manyngardal 'tongue' [maɲŋaɖaɫ]

Pronouns

Pronouns are typologically nominals also, yet their morphosyntactic alignment is nominative–accusative rather than ergative–absolutive.

Nominative Accusative Genitive
1st
person
singular ngagun nganung nganing-gin
dual nginyang nginyang nginyang-gin
plural EXCL ngego ngerreju gerdo-gin
INCL ngego ngerre-ngana gerdo-gin
2nd
person
singular ngigun ngonggo ngonggo-gin
plural ngogo ngorruju gordo-gin
3rd
person
singular ngonggega (rare) nung nung-gin
plural bogo (rare) borruju borro-gin

The 3rd person singular and plural nominative forms, ngonggega and bogo, are labeled 'rare' because they are gradually becoming disused. Speakers prefer to use non-personal pronouns such as gayh- 'that' or gayh-gorden 'those'. Moreover, since the person and number of the subject is contained in the prefix of the verb, nominative free pronouns are often dropped.

Tripartite alignment
Ergative alignment of the case system

While the nominal case system distinguishes the ergative case from absolutive, the free pronouns distinguish nominative from accusative, as shown above. However, they inflect for ergative case as well, resulting in a tripartite case system, as in the following:

ngagun-yi

1SG.NOM-ERG

ngonggo

2SG.ACC

ngany-bu-ng

1sgA.2sgO-hit-PFV

ngagun-yi ngonggo ngany-bu-ng

1SG.NOM-ERG 2SG.ACC 1sgA.2sgO-hit-PFV

'I hit you.'

Accusative alignment of the free pronoun system.
A = Transitive agent
S = Intransitive argument
O = Transitive object

The nominative pronoun root in this instance, ngagun 'I', takes the ergative case suffix -yi to denote the fact that it is the agent of a transitive clause. Conversely, the same pronoun does not take the ergative case when acting as the argument of an intransitive clause:

ngagun

1SG.NOM

maman

good

nga-yu

1SG-be.PRS

ngagun maman nga-yu

1SG.NOM good 1SG-be.PRS

'I am good.'

The accusative pronouns on the other hand, may be accusative or dative, depending on the syntactic requirements of the verb. In the traditional terminology, these pronouns can be either direct or indirect objects.

ngagun-yi

1SG.NOM-ERG

nga-nanda-yi

1sgA.3sgO-see-PST

nung

3SG.ACC

ngagun-yi nga-nanda-yi nung

1SG.NOM-ERG 1sgA.3sgO-see-PST 3SG.ACC

'I saw him/her.'

nga-nawu-ndi

1sgA.3sgO-give-PST

wahan

water

nung

3SG.ACC

nga-nawu-ndi wahan nung

1sgA.3sgO-give-PST water 3SG.ACC

'I gave the water to him/her.'

For these reasons, the pronouns are also labeled base for nominative–ergative pronouns, and oblique for accusative–dative pronouns.

Genitive pronouns

In the table above, genitive pronouns all end with -gin, which is separated orthographically by a hyphen that normally divides morphemes. The -gin form here is not a separate morpheme and cannot be lexically segmented; there is no such word as nganing that would be formed by removing -gin from nganing-gin 'my/mine'. The fact that the genitive forms have regular endings across the entire pronoun paradigm may have been a historical accident.

This cannot be a nominal suffix like those listed above, since it may not attach to other nominals (*warren-gin lari 'the child's hand', but warren-gu lari 'the child's hand'). Furthermore, the genitive pronouns may take a further case suffix, as in the example:

goron

house

nganing-gin-ba

my-ALL

goron nganing-gin-ba

house my-ALL

'To my house.'

This would be prohibited by the restriction against case stacking in Wagiman if the genitive -gin were a case suffix.

Verbs

Verbs are a class of word in Wagiman which contains fewer than 50 members. As it is a closed class, no more verbs are possible. They are often monosyllabic verb roots and all are vowel-final. Wagiman verbs obligatorily inflect for person and number of core arguments, and for the tense and aspect of the clause. A small set of verbs may take a non-finite suffix -yh, in which it may not be further inflected for person or tense. That non-finite verb must then co-occur with another auxiliary verb.

Examples of verbs

Each verb is listed with its past tense marker, which is the second morpheme. Pronunciation given where appropriate.

  • bu-ni 'hit'
  • di-nya 'come'
  • la-ndi 'throw'
  • rinyi-ra 'fall'
  • nanda-yi 'see' [nandaɪ]
  • yu-nginy 'be' [jʊŋɪɲ]

Coverbs

There are so far over 500 recorded coverbs in Wagiman, and more are discovered with continuing research. Compared with verbs, coverbs are far more numerous and far more semantically rich. Verbs express simple, broad meanings such as yu- 'be', ya- 'go' and di- 'come', while coverbs convey more specific, semantically narrow meanings such as barnhbarn-na 'make footprints', lerdongh-nga 'play (a didgeridoo)' or murr-ma 'wade through shallow water using your feet to search for something'.

Coverbs however, cannot inflect for person and cannot, in themselves, head finite clauses. If they are to act as the head of a clause, they must combine with a verb, thereby forming a bipartite verbal compound, commonly called a complex predicate.

Examples of coverbs

Each is listed with the -ma suffix (or its allomorph), which signals aspectual unmarkedness.

  • liri-ma 'swim' [lɪɻɪma]
  • dabaley-ma '(go) around)' [dabalema]
  • gorrh-ma 'fish' [ɡɔrʔma]
  • dippart-ta 'jump' [dɪbˑaɖˑa]
  • wirrnh-na 'whistle' [wɪrʔna]

Syntax

Wagiman is a prefixing language, which, in the context of typology of Australian languages, may refer to its genealogical classification as well as its syntactic properties. Wagiman, along with other Gunwinyguan languages, inflects verbs for person and number of the subject obligatorily, and optionally for the object. In this respect Wagiman displays characteristics of a head-marking language. However, Wagiman also behaves as a dependent-marking language, in that nominals are case marked as to their grammatical or semantic roles, such as ergative (the subject of a transitive clause) or absolutive (the object of a transitive clause or the subject of an intransitive clause).

Morphology

Wagiman is a morphologically rich language and each part of speech has its own set of associated bound morphemes, some of which are obligatory, while others are optional.

Verbs

The verbal prefix contains information about the person and number of the subject, sometimes also the person and number of the object, as well as obligatory information about the tense of the clause. Furthermore, a verbal suffix conveys further information regarding tense and aspect. While only a small number of tense and aspect affixes exist, the interplay between those in the verbal prefix and in the suffix, can generate more highly specified temporal and aspectual clauses.

Further to these affixes, verbs may be marked for the number of the subject, be it dual or plural, and also for clusivity; whether the listener is included in the described event (inclusive) or is excluded from the event (exclusive).

The verbal morphology of tense suffixes in Wagiman is irregular. Of the small inventory of inflecting verbs, many have their own unique tense suffixes, while other tense suffixes are common to several verbs, and while some rudimentary verb classes can be identified - stance verbs always take the past tense suffix -nginy /ŋɪɲ/, for instance - the tense suffixes must be learned for each individual verb.

The prefixes on the other hand, are regular for each verb, although the complete paradigm of verb prefixes is highly complex. They encode three variables: person, number and tense, and are only segmentable in a few cases; one prefix cannot be separated into the three parts. Ngani- for example, encodes second-person singular agent ('you'), first-person singular patient/undergoer ('me') as well as past tense.

ngani-bu-ng

2sgA.1sgO.PST-hit-PFV

ngani-bu-ng

2sgA.1sgO.PST-hit-PFV

'You hit me.'

Nominals

Nominal morphology is significantly less complex than that of the verb. There are a number of case suffixes, denoting ergative, absolutive, dative, allative, locative, ablative, semblative, temporal, instrumental and so on.

Apart from the grammatical cases, ergative and absolutive, which are necessary to construct meaningful sentences, an entire range of semantic cases occur with very high frequency, even when their meaning can be expressed without using case. In the following examples, the former, in which no case is used, is far less common than the latter:

wuji

NEG

nga-nga-gondo-n

IRR-1SG-have-PRS

garradin

money

wuji nga-nga-gondo-n garradin

NEG IRR-1SG-have-PRS money

'I don't have any money.'

garrad-nehen

money-PRIV

nga-yu

1SG.PRS-be

garrad-nehen nga-yu

money-PRIV 1SG.PRS-be

'I am without money.' or 'I am penniless.'

There are also some bound particles, which appear to function in much the same syntactic manner as cases, but which are not considered 'case', for theoretical reasons. -binyju /bɪɲɟʊ/ 'only' is one of these nominal particles, as in:

gubiji-binyju

bone-only

bula-ndi

3SG.leave-PST

gubiji-binyju bula-ndi

bone-only 3SG.leave-PST

'S/he left only the bones.'

Nominals are also marked for number with a suffix that adjoins directly to the root, inside the case suffix. -giwu 'two', for example, would attach to the nominal root before the case, as in:

lamarra-giwu-yi

dog-two-ERG

nganba-badi-na

3plA.1sgO-bite-PST

lamarra-giwu-yi nganba-badi-na

dog-two-ERG 3plA.1sgO-bite-PST

'the two dogs bit me.'

As cases cannot be stacked in Wagiman, these number suffixes cannot be called case suffixes, whereas the nominal suffixes discussed above (such as -binyju 'only'), show the same syntactic distribution - they occur in the same place - and therefore may be analysed as cases themselves.

Coverbs

Coverbs also have their own set of inflectional morphemes, such as aspect, but may also take semantic case suffixes (all those listed above except for ergative and absolutive). For instance, a coverb may take the dative case to convey intention, or purpose, as in:

liri-ma-gu

swim-ASP-DAT

liri-ma-gu

swim-ASP-DAT

'for swimming'

Coverbs are categorially differentiated from nominals though, in that a nominal may not take the aspectual suffixes that a coverb obligatorily takes.

The morpheme that is glossed as aspect in the above example, referred to in the literature as the -ma suffix, denotes aspectual unmarkedness. Its absence signifies perfective aspect, and it may be further suffixed with -yan, producing -ma-yan, to denote continuous or imperfective aspect.

The -ma suffix exhibits regular allomorphy; it assimilates in place and manner of articulation to any preceding obstruent or nasal, but not to any preceding lateral, rhotic or approximant. That is, it remains -ma following vowels, or following the consonants [r], [l], [w] and [j], but when it follows [p], for instance, it assimilates in manner and place, and becomes /-pa/, as in dup-pa 'sit'.

  • liri + maliri-ma
  • wal + mawal-ma
  • bey + mabey-ma
  • yorony + mayorony-nya
  • datj + madatj-ja

The inclusion of the glottal stop in certain words, is ineffective to the surface realisation of the -ma suffix; it will change, or remain unchanged, according to whichever segment precedes the glottal stop, as in:

  • wunh + mawunh-na
  • gayh + magayh-ma

Cross-linguistically, the-ma suffix may be related to a coverbial suffix in Jaminjung, a language in which coverb roots occur without any aspect markers, but are then suffixed with -mayan, which marks continuous aspect. This coverb suffix bears a striking resemblance to the sum of the Wagiman -ma suffix and the continuous aspect suffix -yan, which always occur in tandem on coverbs. Together, -ma and -yan perform the same semantic function as Jaminjung -mayan. Precisely what the relationship holds between these suffixes; whether one language borrowed from the other, or whether each language inherited them from earlier languages, is not at all clear.

Reduplication

Further to derivational and inflectional morphemes, Wagiman coverbs and nominals often undergo reduplication, whereby a part, or often the entirety of the root, is repeated. Reduplication can convey a multitude of meanings. When coverbs are reduplicated, the resulting derived coverb may involve added meaning components such as iterativity, duration or habituality.

dabulp-pa

smoke-ASP

ga-ya

3SG.PRS.go

nu-naw-ma

lots

dabulp-pa ga-ya nu-naw-ma

smoke-ASP 3SG.PRS.go lots

'S/he smokes lots.'

dabuldabulp

smoke.REDUP

ga-ya

3SG.PRS.go

dabuldabulp ga-ya

smoke.REDUP 3SG.PRS.go

'S/he smokes all the time.'

When nominals are derived by reduplication, the added meaning is usually one of plurality. However, since both a dual and a plural nominal suffix exist, -giwu and -guju respectively, nominal reduplication is rare.

Complex predicates

A complex predicate is the combination of more than one element, more than one individual word, to convey the information involved in a single event.[17] For instance, the event swim is conveyed in Wagiman using a combination of a verb ya- 'go' and a coverb liri-ma 'swimming'. There is no verb in Wagiman that, on its own, conveys the event of swimming.

Bipartite verbal compounds such as these are not peculiar to any language in particular. They are in fact very common, and may even occur in every language, albeit with varying frequency. English has a number of complex predicates, include go sightseeing, have breakfast and take (a) bath. The event described by go sightseeing is unable to be described using a single verb sightsee; inflections like sightsaw and sightseen are ungrammatical. An event like take (a) bath, however, may be described by a single verb bathe, but it arguably has a slightly different meaning. Take (a) bath, in any case, is far more common.

Verbalisation

Wagiman is differentiated from other Australian languages in that it has a regular and productive process of verbalisation, whereby coverbs can become verbs and act as the independent head of a clause. Despite being fully productive, meaning that all coverbs may undergo verbalisation, in practice only a handful of coverbs are commonly verbalised. The process appears to be unique to Wagiman within Australian languages.[18]

Verbalisation involves re-analysing the entire coverb - including its suffix -ma, which serves merely to indicate that it is unmarked for aspect - as a verb root, and then to apply the usual obligatory verbal inflection affixes for person, number and tense. As there is no discrete morpheme that serves as a 'verbaliser', the process is one of conversion.[19]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ "Wageman". Ethnologue. Retrieved 10 June 2018.
  2. ^ a b Bowern, C. (2011)
  3. ^ a b c d N27 Wagiman at the Australian Indigenous Languages Database, Australian Institute of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies
  4. ^ Carrington, L., & Triffitt, G. (1999: 266)
  5. ^ a b c Gordon, R. G., Jr. (2005)
  6. ^ a b c Wilson, S. (1999)
  7. ^ a b Merlan, F.C. (1994: 3-4)
  8. ^ Evans, Nicholas (2003), The non-Pama-Nyungan languages of northern Australia : comparative studies of the continent's most linguistically complex region, Pacific Linguistics, p. 14, ISBN 978-0-85883-538-2
  9. ^ Wilson, S. (1999: 5-6)
  10. ^ Harvey, M. (2003) "Verb systems in the Eastern Daly language family." In Nicholas Evans, ed. The Non-Pama-Nyungan languages of northern Australia.
  11. ^ Wilson, A. (2006: 5)
  12. ^ Wilson, S. (1999: 5)
  13. ^ Harvey, M. (2003: 295-97)
  14. ^ Evans, N. (2003: 6-9)
  15. ^ Cook, A.R. (1987: 17-19)
  16. ^ Wilson, S. (2001)
  17. ^ Butt, M. (2003: 2)
  18. ^ Wilson, S. (1999: 82)
  19. ^ Wilson, A. (2006: 14)

References